New neuroscience—as was discussed at the recent Creating a Mindful Society conference—is forcing researchers to take mindful practice seriously. Daniel Goleman reports.
Öser, a
European-born convert to Buddhism, has trained as a Tibetan monk in the
Himalayas for more than three decades, including many years at the side of one
of Tibet’s greatest spiritual masters. But today Öser (whose name has been
changed here to protect his privacy) is about to take a revolutionary step in
the history of the spiritual lineages he has become a part of. He will engage
in meditation while having his brain scanned by state-of-the-art brain imaging
devices.
To be
sure, there have been sporadic attempts to study brain activity in meditators,
and decades of tests with monks and yogis in Western labs, some revealing
remarkable abilities to control respiration, brain waves or core body
temperature. But this—the first experiment with someone at Öser’s level of
training, using such sophisticated measures—will take that research to an
entirely new level. It can take scientists deeper than they have ever been into
charting the specific links between highly disciplined mental strategies and
their impact on brain function. And this research agenda has a pragmatic focus:
to assess meditation as mind training, a practical answer to the perennial
human conundrum of how we can better handle our destructive emotions.
This
issue had been addressed over the course of a remarkable five-day dialogue held
the year before between the Dalai Lama and a small group of scientists at his
private quarters in Dharamsala, India. The research with Öser marked one
culmination of several lines of scientific inquiry set in motion during the
dialogue. There the Dalai Lama had been a prime mover in inspiring this
research; he was an active collaborator in turning the lens of science on the
practices of his own spiritual tradition.
It was at
the invitation of Richard Davidson, one of the scientists who participated in
the Dharamsala dialogues, that Öser had come to the E. M. Keck Laboratory for
Functional Brain Imaging and Behavior, on the Madison campus of the University
of Wisconsin. The laboratory was founded by Davidson, a leading pioneer in the
field of affective neuroscience, which studies the interplay of the brain and
emotions. Davidson had wanted Öser—a particularly intriguing subject—to be
studied intensively with state-of-the-art brain measures.
Öser has
spent several months at a stretch in intensive, solitary retreat. All told,
those retreats add up to about two and a half years. But beyond that, during
several years as the personal attendant to a Tibetan master, the reminders to
practice even in the midst of his busy daily activities were almost constant.
Now, here at the laboratory, the question was what difference any of that
training had made.
The
collaboration began before Öser even went near the MRI, with a meeting to
design the research protocol. As the eight-person research team briefed Öser,
everyone in the room was acutely aware that they were in a bit of a race
against time. The Dalai Lama himself would visit the lab the very next day, and
they hoped by then to have harvested at least some preliminary results to share
with him.
Tibetan
Buddhism may well offer the widest menu of meditation methods of any
contemplative tradition, and it was from this rich offering that the team in
Madison began to choose what to study. The initial suggestions from the
research team were for three meditative states: a visualization, one-pointed
concentration and generating compassion. The three methods involved distinct
enough mental strategies that the team was fairly sure they would reveal
different underlying configurations of brain activity. Indeed, Öser was able to
give precise descriptions of each.
One of
the methods chosen, one-pointedness—a fully focused concentration on a single
object of attention—may be the most basic and universal of all practices, found
in one form or another in every spiritual tradition that employs meditation.
Focusing on one point requires letting go of the ten thousand other thoughts
and desires that flit through the mind as distractions; as the Danish
philosopher Kierkegaard put it, “Purity of heart is to want one thing only.”
In the
Tibetan system (as in many others) cultivating concentration is a beginner’s
method, a prerequisite for moving on to more intricate approaches. In a sense,
concentration is the most generic form of mind training, with many
non-spiritual applications as well. Indeed, for this test, Öser simply picked a
spot (a small bolt above him on the MRI, it turned out) to focus his gaze on,
and held it there, bringing his focus back whenever his mind wandered off.
Öser proposed
three more approaches that he thought would usefully expand the data yield:
meditations on devotion and on fearlessness, and what he called the “open
state.” The last refers to a thought-free wakefulness where the mind, as Öser
described it, “is open, vast and aware, with no intentional mental activity.
The mind is not focused on anything, yet totally present—not in a focused way,
just very open and undistracted. Thoughts may start to arise weakly, but they
don’t chain into longer thoughts—they just fade away.”
Perhaps
as intriguing was Öser’s explanation of the meditation on fearlessness, which
involves “bringing to mind a fearless certainty, a deep confidence that nothing
can unsettle—decisive and firm, without hesitating, where you’re not averse to anything.
You enter into a state where you feel, no matter what happens, ‘I have nothing
to gain, nothing to lose.’”
Focusing
on his teachers plays a key role in the meditation on devotion, he said, in
which he holds in mind a deep appreciation of and gratitude toward his teachers
and, most especially, the spiritual qualities they embody. That strategy also
operates in the meditation on compassion, with his teachers’ kindness offering
a model.
The final
meditation technique, visualization, entailed constructing in the mind’s eye an
image of the elaborately intricate details of a Tibetan Buddhist deity. As Öser
described the process, “You start with the details and build the whole picture
from top to bottom. Ideally, you should be able to keep in mind a clear and
complete picture.” As those familiar with Tibetan thangkas (the wall hangings that depict such
deities) will know, such images are highly complex patterns.
Öser
confidently assumed that each of these six meditation practices should show
distinct brain configurations. The scientists have seen clear distinctions in
cognitive activity between, say, visualization and one-pointedness. But the
meditations on compassion, devotion and fearlessness have not seemed that
different in the mental processes involved, though they differ clearly in
content. From a scientific point of view, if Öser could demonstrate sharp,
consistent brain signatures for any of these meditative states, it would be a
first.
Öser’s
testing started with the “functional MRI,” the current gold standard of
research on the brain’s role in behavior. The standard MRI, in wide use in
hospitals, offers a graphically detailed snapshot of the structure of the
brain. But the fMRI offers all that in video—an ongoing record of how zones of
the brain dynamically change their level of activity from moment to moment. The
conventional MRI lays bare the brain’s structures, while fMRI reveals how those
structures interact as they function.
The fMRI
would give Davidson a crystal-clear set of images of Öser’s brain,
cross-cutting slices at one millimeter—slimmer than a fingernail. These images
could then be analyzed in any dimension to track precisely what happens during
a mental act, tracing paths of activity through the brain.
Öser,
lying peacefully on a hospital gurney with his head constrained in the maw of
the fMRI, looked like a human pencil inserted into a huge cubic beige
sharpener. Instead of the lone monk in a mountaintop cave, it’s the monk in the
brain scanner.
Wearing
earphones so he could listen to the control room, Öser sounded unperturbed as
the technicians led him through a lengthy series of checks to ensure the MRI
images were tracking. Finally, as Davidson was about to begin the protocol, he
asked, “Öser, how are you doing?” “Just fine,” Öser assured him via a small
microphone inside the machine.
“Your
brain looks beautiful,” Davidson said. “Let’s start with five repetitions of
the open state.” A computerized voice then took over, to ensure precise timing
for the protocol. The prompt “on” was the signal for Öser to meditate, followed
by silence for sixty seconds while Öser complied. Then “neutral,” another sixty
seconds of silence, and the cycle started once again with “on.”
The same
routine guided Öser through the other five meditative states, with pauses
between as the technicians worked out various glitches. Finally, when the full
round was complete, Davidson asked if Öser felt the need to repeat any, and the
answer came: “I’d like to repeat the open state, compassion, devotion and
one-pointedness”—the ones he felt were the most important to study.
So the
whole process started again. As he was about to begin the run on the open
state, Öser said he wanted to remain in the state longer. He was able to evoke
the state but wanted more time to deepen it. Once the computers have been
programmed for the protocol, though, the technology drives the procedure; the
timing has been fixed. Still, the technicians went into a huddle, quickly
figuring how to reprogram on the spot to increase the “on” period by fifty
percent and shorten the neutral period accordingly. The rounds began again.
With all
the time taken up by reprogramming and ironing out technical hitches, the whole
run took more than three hours. Subjects rarely emerge from the
MRI—particularly after having been in there for so long—with anything but an
expression of weary relief. But Davidson was pleasantly astonished to see Öser
come out from his grueling routine in the MRI beaming broadly and proclaiming,
“It’s like a mini-retreat!”
Without
taking more than a brief break, Öser headed down the hall for the next set of
tests, this time using an electroencephalogram, the brain wave measure better
known as an EEG. Most EEG studies use only thirty-two sensors on the scalp to
pick up electrical activity in the brain, and many use just six.
But
Öser’s brain would be monitored twice, using two different EEG caps, first one
with 128 sensors, the next with 256. The first cap would capture valuable data
while he again went through the same paces in the meditative states. The
second, with 256 sensors, would be used synergistically with the earlier MRI
data.
This
time, instead of lying in the maw of the MRI, he sat on a comfortable chair and
wore a Medusa-like helmet—something like a shower cap extruding a spaghetti of
thin wires. The EEG sessions took another two hours.
It seemed
from the preliminary analysis that Öser’s mental strategies were accompanied by
strong, demonstrable shifts in the MRI signals. These signals suggested that
large networks in the brain changed with each distinct mental state he
generated. Ordinarily, such a clear shift in brain activity between states of
mind is the exception, except for the grossest shifts in consciousness—from
waking to sleep, for instance. But Öser’s brain showed clear distinctions among
each of the six meditations.
The EEG
analysis bore particularly rich fruit in the comparison between Öser at rest
and while meditating on compassion. Most striking was a dramatic increase in
key electrical activity known as gamma in the left middle frontal gyrus, a zone
of the brain Davidson’s previous research had pinpointed as a locus for
positive emotions. In research with close to two hundred people, Davidson’s lab
had found that when people have high levels of such brain activity in that
specific site of the left prefrontal cortex, they simultaneously report
feelings such as happiness, enthusiasm, joy, high energy and alertness.
On the
other hand, Davidson’s research has also found that high levels of activity in
a parallel site on the other side of the brain—in the right prefrontal
area—correlate with reports of distressing emotions. People with a higher level
of activity in the right prefrontal site and a lower level in the left are more
prone to feelings such as sadness, anxiety and worry. Indeed, an extreme
rightward tilt in the ratio of the activity in these prefrontal areas predicts
a high likelihood that a person will succumb to clinical depression or an
anxiety disorder at some point in their life. People in the grip of depression
who also report intense anxiety have the highest levels of activation in those
right prefrontal areas.
The
implications of these findings for our emotional balance are profound: we each
have a characteristic ratio of right-to-left activation in the prefrontal areas
that offers a barometer of the moods we are likely to feel day to day. That
ratio represents what amounts to an emotional set point, the mean around which
our daily moods swing.
Each of
us has the capacity to shift our moods, at least a bit, and thus change this
ratio. The further to the left that ratio tilts, the better our frame of mind
tends to be, and experiences that lift our mood cause such a leftward tilt, at
least temporarily. For instance, most people show small positive changes in
this ratio when they are asked to recall pleasant memories of events from their
past, or when they watch amusing or heartwarming film clips.
Usually
such changes from the baseline set point are modest. But when Öser was
generating a state of compassion during meditation, he showed a remarkable
leftward shift in this parameter of prefrontal function, one that was
extraordinarily unlikely to occur by chance alone.
In short,
Öser’s brain shift during compassion seemed to reflect an extremely pleasant
mood. The very act of concern for others’ well-being, it seems, creates a
greater state of well-being within oneself. The finding lends scientific
support to an observation often made by the Dalai Lama: that the person doing a
meditation on compassion for all beings is the immediate beneficiary.
The data
from Öser was remarkable in another way, as these were also most likely the
first data ever gathered on brain activity during the systematic generation of
compassion—an emotional state for the most part utterly ignored by modern
psychological research. Research in psychology over the decades has focused far
more on what goes wrong with us—depression, anxiety and the like—than on what
goes right with us. The positive side of experience and human goodness have
been largely ignored in research; indeed, there is virtually no research
anywhere in the annals of psychology on compassion per se.
While
Davidson’s data on compassion were surprising in themselves, still more
remarkable results were about to be reported by Paul Ekman, one of the world’s
most eminent experts on the science of emotion, who heads the Human Interaction
Laboratory at the University of California at San Francisco. Ekman was among
the handful of scientists who had attended the Dharamsala meeting, and he had
studied Öser a few months earlier in his own laboratory. The net result was
four studies, three of which are described here.
The first
test used a measure that represents a culmination of Ekman’s life’s work as the
world’s leading expert on the facial expression of emotions. The test consists
of a videotape in which a series of faces show a variety of expressions very
briefly. The challenge is to identify whether you’ve just seen the facial signs,
for instance, of contempt or anger or fear. Each expression stays on the screen
for just one-fifth of a second in one version, and for one thirtieth of a
second in another—so fast that you would miss it if you blinked. Each time the
person must select which of seven emotions he or she has just seen.
The
ability to recognize fleeting expressions signals an unusual capacity for
accurate empathy. Such expressions of emotion—called micro-expressions—happen
outside the awareness of both the person who displays them and the person
observing. Because they occur unwittingly, these ultra-rapid displays of
emotion are completely uncensored, and so reveal—if only for a short moment—how
the person truly feels.
From
studies with thousands of people, Ekman knew that people who do better at
recognizing these subtle emotions are more open to new experience, more
interested and more curious about things in general. They are also
conscientious—reliable and efficient. “So I had expected that many years of
meditative experience”—which requires both openness and
conscientiousness—“might make them do better on this ability,” Ekman explains.
Thus he had wondered if Öser might be better able to identify these ultra-fast
emotions than other people are.
Then
Ekman announced his results: both Öser and another advanced Western meditator
Ekman had been able to test were two standard deviations above the norm in
recognizing these super-quick facial signals of emotion, albeit the two
subjects differed in the emotions they were best at perceiving. They both
scored far higher than any of the five thousand other people tested. “They do
better than policemen, lawyers, psychiatrists, customs officials, judges—even
Secret Service agents,” the group that had previously distinguished itself as most
accurate.
“It
appears that one benefit of some part of the life paths these two have followed
is becoming more aware of these subtle signs of how other people feel,” Ekman
notes. Öser had super-acuity for the fleeting signs of fear, contempt and
anger. The other meditator—a Westerner who, like Öser, had done a total of two
to three years in solitary retreats in the Tibetan tradition—was similarly
outstanding, though on a different range of emotions: happiness, sadness,
disgust and, like Öser, anger.
One of
the most primitive responses in the human repertoire, the startle reflex,
involves a cascade of very quick muscle spasms in response to a loud,
surprising sound or sudden, jarring sight. For everyone, the same five facial
muscles instantaneously contract during a startle, particularly around the
eyes. The startle reflex starts about two-tenths of a second after hearing the
sound and ends around a half second after the sound. From beginning to end, it
takes approximately a third of a second. The time course is always the same;
that’s the way we’re wired.
Like all
reflexes, the startle reflects activity of the brain stem, the most primitive,
reptilian part of the brain. Like other brain stem responses—and unlike those
of the autonomic nervous system, such as the rate at which the heart beats—the
startle reflex lies beyond the range of voluntary regulation. So far as brain
science understands, the mechanisms that control the startle reflex cannot be
modified by any intentional act.
Ekman
became interested in testing the startle reflex because its intensity predicts
the magnitude of the negative emotions a person feels—particularly fear, anger,
sadness and disgust. The bigger a person’s startle, the more strongly that
individual tends to experience negative emotions—though there’s no relationship
between the startle and positive feelings such as joy.
For a
test of the magnitude of Öser’s startle reflex, Ekman took him across San
Francisco Bay to the psychophysiological laboratory of his colleague Robert
Levenson at the University of California at Berkeley. There they wired Öser to
capture his heart rate and sweat response and videotaped his facial
expressions—all to record his physiological reactions to a startling sound. To
eliminate any differences due to the noise level of the sound, they chose the
top of the threshold for human tolerance to huge sound, like a pistol being
fired or a large firecracker going off near one’s ear.
They gave
Öser the standard instruction, telling him that they would count down from ten
to one, at which point he would hear a loud noise. They asked that he try to
suppress the inevitable flinch, so that someone looking at him would not know
he felt it. Some people can do better than others, but no one can come remotely
close to completely suppressing it. A classic study in the 1940’s showed that
it’s impossible to prevent the startle reflex, despite the most intense,
purposeful efforts to suppress the muscle spasms. No one Ekman and Robert
Levenson had ever tested could do it. Earlier researchers found that even
police marksmen, who fire guns routinely, are unable to keep themselves from
startling.
But Öser
did. Ekman explains, “When Öser tries to suppress the startle, it almost
disappears. We’ve never found anyone who can do that. Nor have any other
researchers.” Öser practiced two types of meditation while having the startle
tested: one-pointed concentration and the open state. As Öser experienced it,
the biggest effect was from the open state: “When I went into the open state,
the explosive sound seemed to me softer, as if I was distanced from the
sensations, hearing the sound from afar.” Ekman reported that although Öser’s
physiology showed some slight changes, not a muscle of his face moved, which
Öser related to his mind not being shaken by the bang. Indeed, as Öser later
elaborated, “If you can remain properly in this state, the bang seems neutral,
like a bird crossing the sky.”
Although
Öser showed not a ripple of movement in any facial muscles while in the open
state, his physiological measures, (including heart rate, sweating and blood
pressure) showed the increase typical of the startle reflex. From Ekman’s
perspective, the strongest overall muting came during the intense focus of the
one-pointedness meditation. During the one-pointedness meditation, instead of
the inevitable jump, there was a decrease in Öser’s heart rate, blood pressure
and so on. On the other hand, his facial muscles did reflect a bit of the
typical startle pattern; the movements “were very small, but they were
present,” Ekman observed. “And he did one unusual thing. In all others we’ve
tested, the eyebrows go down. In Öser they go up.”
In sum,
Öser’s one-pointed concentration seemed to close him off to external
stimuli—even to the startling noise of a gunshot. Given that the larger
someone’s startle, the more intensely that person tends to experience upsetting
emotions, Öser’s performance had tantalizing implications, suggesting a
remarkable level of emotional equanimity.
Finally,
in the last experiment, Ekman and Robert Levenson showed Öser two medical
training films that have been used for more than three decades in emotion
research simply because they are so upsetting. In one a surgeon seems to
amputate a limb with a scalpel and saw—actually preparing an arm stump to be
fitted with a prosthesis—and there is lots of gore and blood. But the camera
focuses only on the limb, so you never see the person getting the surgery. In
the other, you see the pain of a severely burned patient, who stands as doctors
strip skin off his body. The main emotion evoked in the scores of research
subjects who have viewed both these films during experiments is highly
reliable: disgust.
When Öser
viewed the amputation film, the emotion he reported feeling most strongly was
the usual disgust. He commented that the movie reminded him of Buddhist
teachings about impermanence and the unsavory aspects of the human body that
lie beneath an attractive exterior. But his reaction to the burn film was quite
different. “Where he sees the whole person,” Ekman reported, “Öser feels
compassion.” His thoughts were about human suffering and how to relieve it; his
feelings were a sense of caring and concern, mixed with a not unpleasant strong
sadness.
The
physiology of Öser’s disgust reaction during the amputation film was
unremarkable, the standard changes indicating the physiological arousal seen
during that emotion. But when he spontaneously felt compassion during the burn
film, his physiological signs reflected relaxation even more strongly than they
had when the signs had been measured during a resting state.
Ekman
ended his report of the results by noting that each of the studies with Öser
had “produced findings that in thirty-five years of research I have never seen
before.” In short, Öser’s data are extraordinary.
From the
perspective of neuroscience, the point of all this research has nothing to do
with demonstrating that Öser or any other extraordinary person may be
remarkable in him or herself, but rather to stretch the field’s assumptions
about human possibility.
A decade
ago the dogma in neuroscience was that the brain contained all of its neurons
at birth and it was unchanged by life’s experiences. The only changes that
occurred over the course of life were minor alterations in synaptic
contacts—the connections among neurons—and cell death with aging. But the new
watchword in brain science is neuroplasticity, the notion that the brain
continually changes as a result of our experiences—whether through fresh
connections between neurons or through the generation of utterly new neurons.
Musical training, where a musician practices an instrument every day for years,
offers an apt model for neuroplasticity. MRI studies find that in a violinist,
for example, the areas of the brain that control finger movements in the hand
that does the fingering grow in size. Those who start their training earlier in
life and practice longer show bigger changes in the brain. Still,
neuroscientists do not know with certainty what accounts for this change—whether
the change is in the synaptic weights as added connections bulk out neurons, or
whether an uptick in the number of neurons may also be playing a role.
A related
issue revolves around the amount of practice that it might take in order for
the brain to show such a change, particularly in something as subtle as
meditation. There is an undeniable impact on the brain, mind and body from
extensive practice. Studies of champion performers in a range of abilities—from
chess masters and concert violinists to Olympic athletes—find pronounced
changes in the pertinent muscle fibers and cognitive abilities that set those
at the top of a skill apart from all others.
The more
total hours of practice the champions have done, the stronger the changes. For
instance, among violinists at the topmost level, all had practiced a lifetime
total of about ten thousand hours by the time they entered a music academy.
Those at the next rung had practiced an average of about seventy-five-hundred
hours. Presumably a similar effect from practice occurs in meditation, which
can be seen, from the perspective of cognitive science, as the systematic
effort to retrain attention and related mental and emotional skills.
Öser, as
it turned out, far exceeded the ten-thousand-hour level in meditation practice.
Much of that practice came during the time he spent in intensive meditation
retreats, along with the four years living in a hermitage during the early
period of his training as a monk, as well as occasional long retreats over the
subsequent years.
While
Öser may be a virtuoso of meditation, even raw novices start to show some of
the same shifts. This was clear from other data Davidson had gathered on
similar brain changes in people just beginning to practice a variety of
meditation called mindfulness. These studies had given Davidson convincing data
that meditation can shift the brain as well as the body. While Öser’s results
suggested just how far that shift could go with years of sustained practice,
even beginners displayed evidence of biological shifts in the same direction.
So the next question for Davidson to tackle was this: can specific types of
meditation be used to change circuitry in the brain associated with different
aspects of emotion?
Davidson
may be one of the few neuroscientists anywhere who can dare to ask this,
because his lab is using a new imaging technique—diffusion tensor imaging—to
help answer this question. The method shows connections among different regions
in the nervous system. Until now, diffusion tensor imaging has mostly been used
to study patients with neurological diseases. Davidson’s lab is among a select
group that use the technique for basic neuroscience research, and the only one
to be using it for research on how methods that transform emotion may be
changing the connectivity of the brain.
Perhaps
most exciting, the images created by diffusion tensor imaging can actually
track the subtle reshaping of the brain at the heart of neuroplasticity. With
the method, scientists can now, identify the changes in the human brain as
repeated experiences remodel specific connections or add new neurons. This
marks a brave new frontier for neuroscience: it was only in 1998 that
neuroscientists discovered that new neurons are continually being generated in
the adult brain.
For
Davidson, one immediate application will be searching for new connections in
the circuitry crucial for regulating distressing emotions. Davidson hopes to
see if there actually are new connections associated with a person’s increased
ability to manage anxiety, fear or anger more effectively.
From the
scientific perspective, what does any of this matter? Davidson sums it up by
referring to The Art of
Happiness, a book the Dalai Lama wrote with psychiatrist Howard Cutler, in
which the Dalai Lama said that happiness is not a fixed characteristic, a
biological set point that will never change. Instead, the brain is plastic, and
our quota of happiness can be enhanced through mental training.
“It can
be trained because the very structure of our brain can be modified,” Davidson
said. “And the results of modern neuroscience inspire us now to go on and look
at other practiced subjects so that we can examine these changes with more
detail. We now have the methods to show how the brain changes with these kinds
of practices, and how our mental and physical health may improve as a
consequence.”
Öser,
reflecting on the data gathered in Madison, put it this way: “Such results of
training point to the possibility that one could continue much further in such
a transformation process, and, as some great contemplatives have repeatedly
claimed, eventually free one’s mind from afflictive emotions.”
When I
asked the Dalai Lama what he made of the data on Öser—such as being able to
mute the startle reflex—he replied, “It’s very good he managed to show some
signs of yogic ability.” Here he used the term yogic not in the garden-variety
sense of a few hours a week practicing postures in a yoga studio but in its
classic sense—referring to one who dedicates his or her life to the cultivation
of spiritual qualities.
The Dalai
Lama added, “But there is a saying, ‘The true mark of being learned is humility
and mental discipline; the true mark of a meditator is that he has disciplined
his mind by freeing it from negative emotions.’ We think along those lines—not
in terms of performing some feats or miracles.” In other words, the real
measure of spiritual development lies in how well a person manages disturbing
emotions such as anger and jealousy—not in attaining rarified states during meditation
or exhibiting feats of physical self-control such as muting the startle
reaction.
One
payoff for this scientific agenda would be in inspiring people to better handle
their destructive emotions through trying some of the same methods for training
the mind. When I asked the Dalai Lama what greater benefit he hoped for from
this line of research, he replied: “Through training the mind people can become
more calm—especially those who suffer from too many ups and downs. That’s the
conclusion from these studies of Buddhist mind training. And that’s my main
end: I’m not thinking how to further Buddhism, but how the Buddhist tradition
can make some contribution to the benefit of society. Of course, as Buddhists,
we always pray for all sentient beings. But we’re only human beings; the main
thing you can do is train your own mind.”
No comments:
Post a Comment